Generic selectors
Exact matches only
Search in title
Search in content
Post Type Selectors
Filter by Categories
Case Report
Editorial
Erratum
Guest Editorial
Letter to Editor
Letter to the Editor
Media and News
Medial Education
Medical Education
Obituary
Opinion Article
Original Article
Review Article
Short Communication
Generic selectors
Exact matches only
Search in title
Search in content
Post Type Selectors
Filter by Categories
Case Report
Editorial
Erratum
Guest Editorial
Letter to Editor
Letter to the Editor
Media and News
Medial Education
Medical Education
Obituary
Opinion Article
Original Article
Review Article
Short Communication
Generic selectors
Exact matches only
Search in title
Search in content
Post Type Selectors
Filter by Categories
Case Report
Editorial
Erratum
Guest Editorial
Letter to Editor
Letter to the Editor
Media and News
Medial Education
Medical Education
Obituary
Opinion Article
Original Article
Review Article
Short Communication
View/Download PDF

Translate this page into:

Short Communication
ARTICLE IN PRESS
doi:
10.25259/IJPP_292_2024

Self-reported impulsivity and mood states predict cue-induced food craving in adults with obesity: A cross-sectional study

Patanjali Research Foundation Trust, Haridwar, Uttarakhand, India.

*Corresponding author: Shirley Telles, Patanjali Research Foundation Trust, Patanjali Yogpeeth Phase 1, Haridwar, Uttarakhand, India. office@prft.co.in

Licence
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-Non Commercial-Share Alike 4.0 License, which allows others to remix, transform, and build upon the work non-commercially, as long as the author is credited and the new creations are licensed under the identical terms.

How to cite this article: Sharma SK, Gupta A, Telles S, Balkrishna A. Self-reported impulsivity and mood states predict cue-induced food craving in adults with obesity: A cross-sectional study. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. doi: 10.25259/IJPP_292_2024

Abstract

Objective:

To determine the relationship of mood state and impulsivity with cue-induced food craving.

Materials and Methods:

Forty obese adults (group average age±SD; 42.68±11.62 years) participated in this cross-sectional study. Each participant was assessed for (i) positive and negative mood states, (ii) impulsivity, and (iii) cue-induced food craving (FC).

Results:

Linear regression analysis showed a significant negative association between a more positive and less negative mood state and cue-induced FC, while a positive association between impulsivity and FC.

Conclusion:

A more positive and less negative mood state is negatively associated with FC, whereas state impulsivity is positively associated with FC in obese persons.

Keywords

Obese adults
Addiction-like eating behaviours
State food craving
State impulsivity
Mood state
Multiple linear regressions

INTRODUCTION

Addiction-like eating behaviours have been frequently reported as a major contributing factor to overeating and weight gain.[1] Food craving is one such addiction-like eating behaviour characterised by an intense and frequent desire to consume specific foods that are particularly high in fat and sugar with low nutritional value.[2-6] Food craving was positively associated with uncontrolled eating and emotional eating in a large sample (n = 1065) of adults.[7] A quantitative predictive meta-analysis of 45 studies involving 3292 participants concluded a moderate association of food craving with food cue reactivity and body mass index (BMI) (r = 0.33, 11% variance).[8] With this, there is an increasing interest in understanding the behavioural factors that are associated with or contribute to food cravings.[9]

A negative mood state has been linked to substance and behavioural addictions.[10] For example, a systematic review and meta-analysis of 14 studies, including 2257 participants, concluded a moderate association between negative mood states and craving for substance use.[10] Apart from this, a negative mood state has been positively correlated with hedonic eating or eating for pleasure.[11]

The association of negative mood states with substance and behavioural addictions has been attributed to impulsivity.[12] Impulsivity refers to the tendency to respond without full consideration of consequences in situations where a thoughtful response is more appropriate.[13] A review of empirical evidence examining how negative mood states perpetuate substance addictions reported that impulsivity modulates the effect of negative mood states on substance and behavioural addictions.[12] Apart from this, impulsivity has been linked to poor eating habits. For example, higher self-reported impulsivity was associated with unhealthy food choices, overeating and eating for emotional reasons.[14,15]

Considering that both negative mood states and impulsivity have been linked to substance and behavioural addictions and that food craving resembles addictive behaviour, the present study aimed at determining the association of (i) self-perceived state impulsivity and (ii) mood state with cue-induced food craving in obese adults. Pictures of two energy-dense food items consumed in India (i.e., samosa, a deep-fried savoury pastry with rich fillings and jalebi, a sweet pastry fried and served in sugar syrup) were used to induce state food cravings during the assessment of food cravings in the present study.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Forty obese adults (BMI ≥25 kg/m2)[16] of both genders (M: F = 23:17) were recruited from a wellness centre in north India. Recruitment was carried out by announcements in the wellness centre. There was no incentive to take part in the study. Participants were included if they (i) were aged between 20 and 60 years and (ii) had a BMI ≥25 kg/m2.[16] Participants were to be excluded if they had (i) a History of type 2 diabetes mellitus, (ii) regular medication which influences hunger and satiety and (iii) regular consumption of alcohol or tobacco (in any form). None of the participants were excluded due to these reasons. The baseline characteristics of the participants are given in Table 1. A signed informed consent form was obtained from each participant. The study had prior approval from the Institutional Ethics Committee (number PRF/YRD/22/019).

Table 1: The baseline characteristics of the participants.
S. No. Characteristics No. of participants (n=40)
1. Group average age 42.68±11.62
2. Male: Female 23 : 17
3. Anthropometric measures
3.1.   BMI (kg/m2) 29.82±4.48
4. FCQ-S
4.1.   Total FCQ-S scores 46.1±13.61
4.2.   Desire to eat 7.56±4.07
4.3.   Positive reinforcement 17.69±5.3
4.4.   Negative relief 7.49±2.9
4.5.   Lack of control 8.46±3.87
4.6.   Craving/hunger as physiological state 8.51±3.74
5. STIMP
5.1.   Impulsivity and control of behaviour 18.01±8.09
5.2.   Control of thoughts 12±9.4
5.3.   Feeling of impatience 10.92±9.47
5.4.   Total STIMP 40.93±20.43
6. BMIS
6.1.   Pleasant-unpleasant mood score 52.85±8.51
6.2.   Aroused-calm mood score 30.82±3.75
6.3.   Positive-tired mood score 23.56±3.3
6.4.   Negative-relaxed mood score 10.33±4.41

BMIS: Brief mood introspection scale, STIMP: State impulsivity scales, BMI: Body mass index, FCQ-S: Food cravings questionnaire-State

Study design

This cross-sectional study was designed to determine the association of cue-induced food craving with (i) self-reported impulsivity and (ii) mood states in obese adults.

Assessments

Food craving state

State food craving was measured using the original version of the Food Cravings Questionnaire-State (FCQ-S) developed by Cepeda-Benito et al.[17-19] The FCQ-S has 15 items as statements that describe the intensity of food craving state on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. Each item of the FCQ-S is given a weighted score of 1–5. The possible scores range between 15 and 75, where higher scores represent an overall higher intensity of state food craving. Pictures of two energy-dense food items consumed in India (i.e., samosa, a deep-fried savoury pastry with rich fillings and jalebi, a sweet pastry fried and served in sugar syrup) were used to induce state food craving during the administration of the FCQ-S in the present study.

Self-reported state impulsivity

State impulsivity was assessed using the state impulsivity scale.[20] The scale consists of 14 visual analogue scales that are designed to measure impulsive mood states.

Mood state

Positive and tired mood state was assessed using the positive-tired mood sub-scale of the brief mood introspection scale (BMIS).[21] The sub-scale consists of seven mood adjectives. Of these, five items are related to positive mood states (i.e., lively, caring, peppy, loving and active), while the remaining two items are related to negative mood states (i.e., tired and drowsy). The participants were asked to rate the intensity of each mood adjective on a four-point rating scale. The higher scores on the scale show greater positive and less tired mood states.

Data analysis

Multiple linear regression analyses using a forward conditional selection criterion (with a probability-of-F-to-enter ≤0.05) were carried out using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (Version 24.0) to determine the association of self-perceived state impulsivity scores and the positive-tired mood sub-scale scores of the BMIS with food craving state. Positive-tired mood sub-scale scores of the BMIS and self-perceived state impulsivity scores were entered in the first and second steps, respectively, in the model. Tolerance values and variance inflation values for the independent variables in the final regression models were examined to determine the collinearity. Tolerance values of <0.1 and variance inflation factor values of more than 5 were considered to suggest problematic collinearity. No problems with collinearity were found within the analyses carried out. Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05 level.

RESULTS

Forty obese male and female participants (m: f = 23:17) aged between 42.68 ± 11.62 years completed the study. The baseline characteristics of the participants are provided in Table 1.

Self-perceived state impulsivity scores were significantly associated with state food craving (P = 0.002; 22.7% of variance). Furthermore, positive-tired mood sub-scale scores of the BMIS were significantly associated with state food craving (P = 0.037; 8.7% of variance). The results of the final steps of the multiple regression analyses are presented in Table 2.

Table 2: Results for step-wise multiple linear regression analysis.
Dependent variable Step Step change in R2 P-value for step change in R2 Beta for final model (only significant predictor variables shown) P-value
State impulsivity questionnaire scores First 0.227 0.002 0.399 0.007
Positive-tired subscale scores of the brief mood introspection scale Second 0.087 0.037 −0.305 0.037

Adjusted R2 for model=0.277

DISCUSSION

The purpose of the present study was to examine the association of mood state and self-perceived state impulsivity with state food craving in obese persons. The results showed a negative association of state food craving with more positive and less negative mood states (Beta = −0.305), while a positive association between self-perceived impulsivity and state food craving (Beta = 0.399).

The negative association of more positive and less negative mood states with food cravings in the present sample of obese persons is consistent with previous reports on addictive behaviours.[22] For example, smokers with a negative mood state had a higher self-reported craving for cigarettes compared to those with a neutral mood state.[22]

Furthermore, the findings of the present study (i.e., a positive association of state food craving with state impulsivity) resemble the results of the previous studies on substance and behaviour addictions.[13] For example, a recent systematic meta-review of six meta-analyses that examined neurocognitive functions central to impulsive-compulsive behaviours across substance and behavioural addictions concluded that impulsivity plays an important role in both types of addiction.[13]

It has been hypothesised that a negative mood state increases impulsivity, which, in turn, leads to addictive behaviours.[12] This may explain the association of food craving with mood states and impulsivity in this study, as food craving resembles addictive behaviour.

Given that mood states influence impulsivity, the findings of this study suggest that dietary interventions for obesity should include strategies to improve mood states, as a positive mood state is associated with low cue-induced state food cravings. This may prevent weight gain resulting from overeating associated with state food cravings.

The findings of the present study are limited by the small sample size and that the assessments were based on self-reports. To address these limitations, future research should focus on different dimensions of impulsivity and mood states.

CONCLUSION

A more positive and less negative mood state is negatively associated with food craving, whereas state impulsivity is positively associated with food craving in obese persons.

Ethical approval

The research/study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Patanjali Research Foundation (Trust), number PRF/YRD/22/019, dated 07 September 2023.

Declaration of patient consent

The authors certify that they have obtained all appropriate patient consent.

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

Use of artificial intelligence (AI)-assisted technology for manuscript preparation

The authors confirm that there was no use of artificial intelligence (AI)-assisted technology for assisting in the writing or editing of the manuscript and no images were manipulated using AI.

Financial support and sponsorship

Nil.

References

  1. , , , , , , et al. Effects of addictive-like eating behaviors on weight loss with behavioral obesity treatment. J Behav Med. 2019;42:246-55.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. , , . Cue-elicited craving for food: A fresh approach to the study of binge eating. Appetite. 2005;44:253-61.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. , . Psychometric properties of the food craving inventory among obese patients with binge eating disorder. Eat Behav. 2005;6:239-45.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. , , , . Food cravings, food intake, and weight status in a community-based sample. Eat Behav. 2014;15:478-82.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. , , , , , , et al. Food Cravings Questionnaire-Trait (FCQ-T) discriminates between obese and overweight patients with and without binge eating tendencies: The Italian version of the FCQ-T. J Pers Assess. 2014;96:632-9.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. , . The body asks and the mind judges: Food cravings in eating disorders. Encephale. 2020;46:269-82.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. , , , . The role of craving in emotional and uncontrolled eating. Appetite. 2018;123:146-51.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. , . Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: A meta-analytic review. Obes Rev. 2016;17:159-77.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. , , , , . Immune responses of goats persistently infected with caprine arthritis-encephalitis virus. Infect Immun. 1980;28:421-7.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. , , , , . Association between negative affectivity and craving in substance-related disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis of direct and indirect relationships. Psychol Rep. 2023;126:1143-80.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. , , , , , , et al. The association of emotional eating with overweight/obesity, depression, anxiety/stress, and dietary patterns: A review of the current clinical evidence. Nutrients. 2023;15:1173.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. . How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2001;158:343-59.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. , , . A systematic meta-review of impulsivity and compulsivity in addictive behaviors. Neuropsychol Rev. 2019;29:14-26.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. , . Impulsive reactivity to emotion and vulnerability to psychopathology. Am Psychol. 2018;73:1067-78.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. , , , . Impulsivity and fast-food consumption: A cross-sectional study among working adults. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2016;116:61-8.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. , , . Assessment of obesity in the Indian setting: A clinical review. Clin Epidemiol Global Health. 2023;23:101348.
    [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  17. , , , , , . The development and validation of Spanish versions of the state and trait food cravings questionnaires. Behav Res Ther. 2000;38:1125-38.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. , , , . The development and validation of the state and trait food-cravings questionnaires. Behav Ther. 2000;31:151-73.
    [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  19. . The psychology of food cravings: The role of food deprivation. Curr Nutr Rep. 2020;9:251-7.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. , . Impulsivity: A state as well as trait variable. Does mood awareness explain low correlations between trait and behavioural measures of impulsivity? Pers Individ Dif. 1997;22:333-9.
    [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  21. , . The experience and meta-experience of mood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;55:102-11.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. , , , , , . Sadness, but not all negative emotions, heightens addictive substance use. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020;117:943-9.
    [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Show Sections

Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology

Copyright Form


Title of the Manuscript: ________________________________________


I/We certify that I/we have participated sufficiently in the intellectual content, conception, and design of this work, or the analysis and interpretation of the data (when applicable), as well as the writing of the manuscript, to take public responsibility for it. I/We agree to have my/our name(s) listed as contributors and confirm that the manuscript represents valid work.

Each author confirms they meet the criteria for authorship as established by the ICMJE. Neither this manuscript nor one with substantially similar content under my/our authorship has been published or is being considered for publication elsewhere, except as described in the covering letter.

I/We certify that all data collected during the study is presented in this manuscript and that no data from the study has been or will be published separately. I/We agree to provide, upon request by the editors, any data/information on which the manuscript is based for examination by the editors or their assignees.

I/We have disclosed all financial interests, direct or indirect, that exist or may be perceived to exist for individual contributors in connection with the content of this manuscript in the cover letter. Sources of outside support for the project are also disclosed in the cover letter.

In accordance with open access principles, I/we grant the Journal the exclusive right to publish and distribute this work under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike (CC BY-NC-SA) license. This license permits others to distribute, transform, adapt, and build upon the material in any medium or format for non-commercial purposes, provided appropriate credit is given to the creator(s). Any adaptations must be shared under the same license terms. The key elements of the CC BY-NC-SA license are:

  • BY: Credit must be given to the original creator(s).
  • NC: Only non-commercial uses of the work are permitted.
  • SA: Adaptations must be shared under the same license terms.

I/We retain academic rights to the material, and the Journal is authorized to:

  1. Grant permission to republish the article in whole or in part, with or without fee.
  2. Produce preprints or reprints and translate the work into other languages for sale or free distribution.
  3. Republish the work in a collection of articles in any mechanical or electronic format.

I/We give the rights to the corresponding author to make necessary changes as requested by the Journal, handle all correspondence on our behalf, and act as the guarantor for the manuscript.

All individuals who have made substantial contributions to the work but do not meet the criteria for authorship are named in the Acknowledgment section with their written permission. If no acknowledgment is provided, it signifies that no substantial contributions were made by non-authors.


Name of the author(s) Signature Date signed Corresponding author?
Yes/No
Yes/No
Yes/No